Most of Us Combine Personality Traits from Different Genders

Sign up for Scientific American’s free newsletters.

” =”articleBody” name=”articleBody”>

How different are men and women really? About 30 years ago, if dating guides are any indication, some people assumed vast differences in personality, with Men Are from Mars, Women Are from Venus relationship advice. Today, by contrast, certain communities are pushing back against the idea of binary gender, which presents men and women as separate categories. (A quick reminder: gender—described in terms such as “man,” “woman” and “nonbinary person”—has strong cultural and social components, whereas sex—sometimes described with adjectives such as “male,” “female” and “intersex”—refers to a combination of biological features.)

Scientists are also raising questions around gender, sex and personality. For one, researchers debate how big the personality differences between cisgender men and women truly are. The answer depends on how psychologists measure an individual’s characteristics. For example, a study involving more than 300,000 people in the U.S., who self-identified as either male or female looked both at broad personality traits, such as extraversion and agreeableness, and more specific aspects of these traits, such as a warm communication style or tendency to act altruistically. In this sample, researchers found moderate differences between male and female participants in the broad traits but, in most cases, larger differences at the more specific level. For instance, overall, both male and female individuals in the study were similarly extraverted (a broad trait). When the scientists looked at specific aspects of extraversion, however, they found that male participants, on average, sought out exciting situations more often than female ones did. And female participants demonstrated higher activity levels than male ones.

Researchers also argue about whether it makes more sense to study the size of personality differences by looking at one isolated characteristic at a time or by considering all traits simultaneously. The latter approach tends to produce substantially larger differences, much the way that male and female faces don’t differ much if you look just at their eyes or nose but become easier to distinguish if you look at the whole face.

At ClearerThinking.org, a project that investigates the psychology of happiness and decision-making, we wanted to contribute to this discussion and help the public learn more about sex, gender and personality. We ran a series of 15 studies and conducted analyses on more than 15,000 people with the goal of testing the most comprehensive set of personality traits to date. Our analyses are limited to cisgender men and women because of our sample: 98 percent of our participants identified as either a “man” or “woman” and reported that this gender identity aligned with their sex assigned at birth. We therefore did not have enough data to shed light on the personality traits of nonbinary and transgender people—so although many individuals fall into these other categories, our research focused on cisgender men and women.

From the data, we discovered 18 specific self-reported traits that varied between these men and women. Next, we built an algorithm designed to predict a person’s gender based on their self-reported scores on those traits, which was accurate 78 percent of the time. That accuracy is high but far from perfect, revealing the challenge of predicting an individual’s gender from their combination of traits. Finally, we adapted our study questionnaire—in which people rated how much they agree with statements such as “I laugh aloud” and “I frequently worry”—into an online interactive assessment. You can try the quiz yourself to see how well the algorithm predicts your gender.

To create the questions for our studies, we cast a very wide net, looking at large personality projects, reviewing the academic literature and crowdsourcing ideas. We ended up testing more than 600 personality questions for gender differences before identifying the 18 traits with the greatest variation between the self-identified men and women in our sample. These traits included not only the broad characteristics that are widely used in psychological research (such as extraversion and agreeableness) but also more specific patterns of thought and behavior, such as how frequently an individual takes risks or their degree of focus on aesthetics. We also double-checked our conclusions by running a final study to replicate the major findings. Ultimately, we found no large differences in personality between cisgender men and women on any traits. But we did find small- and moderate-sized differences in the 18 personality traits.

The largest difference we detected was the degree to which cisgender people thought about sex, assessed by asking people to rate how much they agreed with the statement “I often have sexual thoughts when I meet an attractive looking person” and disagreed with the statement “I do not frequently think about sex.” (This “sex-focused” characteristic, while not linked to major personality traits commonly studied in psychology, nonetheless fits the conception of a personality trait as a pattern in thought, emotion or behavior. Furthermore, it relates to a concept called sexual preoccupation.) We found that gender could explain about 18 percent of the variation in the extent to which people are sex-focused. Men had a higher average score on this trait than women. There were still plenty of women who had a higher score than most men, however. In other words, individual men and women were highly varied, even though, at the group level, men tended to differ from women. We also found that, on average, men’s self-reported personality was a bit more thick-skinned, risk-taking and self-valuing. In contrast, on average, women’s self-reported personality was a bit more unselfish, compassionate and peaceful.

On every trait, there was a substantial overlap between men and women. Yet at the tail ends—where people either strongly agreed or disagreed with the questions we asked them—larger differences emerged. For example, very low compassion was rare in both men and women, but the few people who identified as very uncompassionate were much more likely to be men. This result is consistent with the finding that antisocial personality disorder, which often involves a lack of remorse or empathy, is more common among men than women.

So is there a “man’s personality” and a “woman’s personality”? Fascinatingly, almost everyone in our study was a mix of “more often seen in men” and “more often seen in women” traits. For any given trait, an individual woman was closer to the overall average for women than the overall average for men just 61 percent of the time. And a man was closer to the average for men than the average for women only 57 percent of the time. Only about 1 percent of men and 1 percent of women had almost entirely more-often-seen-in-men or more-often-seen-in-women personality traits. Accordingly, because nearly everyone is a mix of both, we named the personality assessment we had created from this research the Gender Continuum Test.

To test how accurately gender can be predicted from personality, we developed a simple machine-learning algorithm (a computer program that looked for patterns in data regarding which personality traits are associated with being a cis man or cis woman). We trained our algorithm using results from past study participants, then presented the algorithm with the personality traits of new participants to see how well it could predict their gender. Using just the most predictive trait—being sex-focused—the algorithm could predict a person’s gender correctly 69 percent of the time. This result may be impressive to some. But the prediction is far from perfect because some women are much more sex-focused than the average man.

The algorithm’s accuracy rose to 78 percent when we allowed it to incorporate all the personality differences at once. That’s a big improvement—but for the other 22 percent of people, the algorithm was predicting incorrectly. When we released our quiz to the public, accuracy slipped a bit further to 74 percent. That’s still much better than the average human, though: We gave another group of study participants sets of personality traits that, we explained, belonged to particular individuals. Then we asked the participants to predict the gender of those other people using the personality traits. They were correct only 58 percent of the time, hardly better than a coin flip.

We believe our results shed new light on the size of gender differences in personality. There are, however, some important caveats to acknowledge. First, all of our study participants were from the U.S., and given that factors such as culture influence personality and gender, we would be hesitant to extend our conclusions to other communities. Second, our study cannot provide insight into the causes of personality differences—for instance, how much these differences can be explained by environment and culture as opposed to biology. Third, as noted, given our pool of participants, we don’t have enough data to comment on transgender, intersex or nonbinary individuals. We hope future research explores these and other dimensions of the personality, sex and gender debate.

In the meantime, our study is a reminder that, on average, cisgender men and women do have some small to moderate differences in how they report their personality, but almost everyone is a mix of more-often-seen-in-men and more-often- seen-in-women traits. If you try to guess someone’s personality from their gender, you’ll very often be wrong.

Are you a scientist who specializes in neuroscience, cognitive science or psychology? And have you read a recent peer-reviewed paper that you would like to write about for Mind Matters? Please send suggestions to Scientific American’s Mind Matters editor Daisy Yuhas at pitchmindmatters@gmail.com.

Note: This article have been indexed to our site. We do not claim legitimacy, ownership or copyright of any of the content above. To see the article at original source Click Here

Related Posts
These 3,000-year-old relics were torched and buried—but why? thumbnail

These 3,000-year-old relics were torched and buried—but why?

History & CultureOut of Eden WalkBug-eyed sculptures. Huge masks of hammered gold. These artifacts in China have long puzzled archaeologists—but may show us how to adapt to a changing world.Published January 27, 202314 min readWriter and National Geographic Society Explorer Paul Salopek’s Out of Eden Walk is a 24,000-mile storytelling odyssey across the world in
Read More
New species of marine plankton discovered, an overlooked source of nutrients in the oceans thumbnail

New species of marine plankton discovered, an overlooked source of nutrients in the oceans

A team of University of Hawai'i at Manoa researchers in the Department of Oceanography's Center for Microbial Oceanography -- Research and Education (C-MORE), along with collaborators at the University of California Santa Cruz, and the California State University San Marcos, have just published a paper in Nature Communications announcing the discovery of two new and…
Read More
Megalodon Grew to Larger Sizes in Cooler Waters: Study thumbnail

Megalodon Grew to Larger Sizes in Cooler Waters: Study

Otodus megalodon, the largest shark that ever lived on Earth, most certainly reached at least 15 m (50 feet) in total length based on its gigantic teeth. New research demonstrates that individuals of Otodus megalodon were on average larger in cooler water than those in warmer water. Shimada et al. suggest that the notion that…
Read More
Index Of News
Consider making some contribution to keep us going. We are donation based team who works to bring the best content to the readers. Every donation matters.
Donate Now

Subscription Form

Liking our Index Of News so far? Would you like to subscribe to receive news updates daily?

Total
0
Share