The Secrets Of Cooperation

People stop their cars simply because a little light turns from green to red. They crowd onto buses, trains and planes with complete strangers, yet fights seldom break out. Large, strong men routinely walk right past smaller, weaker ones without demanding their valuables. People pay their taxes and donate to food banks and other charities.

Most of us give little thought to these everyday examples of cooperation. But to biologists, they’re remarkable — most animals don’t behave that way.

“Even the least cooperative human groups are more cooperative than our closest cousins, chimpanzees and bonobos,” saysMichael Muthukrishnaa behavioral scientist at the London School of Economics. Chimps don’t tolerate strangers, Muthukrishna says, and even young children are a lot more generous than a chimp.

Human cooperation takes some explaining — after all, people who act cooperatively should be vulnerable to exploitation by others. Yet in societies around the world, people cooperate to their mutual benefit. Scientists are making headway in understanding the conditions that foster cooperation, research that seems essential as an interconnected world grapples with climate change, partisan politics and more — problems that can be addressed only through large-scale cooperation.

Behavioral scientists’ formal definition of cooperation involves paying a personal cost (for example, contributing to charity) to gain a collective benefit (a social safety net). But freeloaders enjoy the same benefit without paying the cost, so all else being equal, freeloading should be an individual’s best choice — and, therefore, we should all be freeloaders eventually.

Many millennia of evolution acting on both our genes and our cultural practices have equipped people with ways of getting past that obstacle, says Muthukrishna, who coauthored a look at theevolution of cooperationin the 2021Annual Review of Psychology.This cultural-genetic coevolution stacked the deck in human society so that cooperation became the smart move rather than a sucker’s choice. Over thousands of years, that has allowed us to live in villages, towns and cities; work together to build farms, railroads and other communal projects; and develop educational systems and governments.

Evolution has enabled all this by shaping us to value the unwritten rules of society, to feel outrage when someone else breaks those rules and, crucially, to care what others think about us.

“Over the long haul, human psychology has been modified so that we’re able to feel emotions that make us identify with the goals of social groups,” saysRob Boydan evolutionary anthropologist at the Institute for Human Origins at Arizona State University.

For a demonstration of this, one need look no further than a simple lab experiment that psychologists call the dictator game. In this game, researchers give a sum of money to one person (the dictator) and tell them they can split the money however they’d like with an unknown other person whom they will never meet. Even though no overt rule prohibits them from keeping all the money themselves, many people’s innate sense of fairness leads them tosplit the money 50-50. Cultures differ in how often this happens, but even societies where the sense of fairness is weakest still choose a fair split fairly often.

(Credit: Knowable Magazine) Researchers offered a reward, such as money, to volunteers from eight different societies, then offered them the chance to give half to another person they didn’t know and would never meet. Before making a decision, participants watched one of three videos in which an adult from their community expressed whether sharing or not-sharing was good or bad, or if both were OK. People chose to share more often in societies that judged it as correct. Societies tested were four industrialized cities: Berlin (Germany), La Plata (Argentina), Phoenix (Arizona) and Pune (India) and four small-scale traditional societies: the Shuar (Ecuador), Wichí (Argentina), Tanna (Vanuatu) and Hadza (Tanzania).

Lab experiments such as this, together with field studies, are giving psychologists a better understanding of the psychological factors that underpin when, and why, people cooperate. Here are some of the essential takeaways:

We cooperate for different reasons at different social scales

For very small groups, family bonds and direct reciprocity — I’ll help you today, on the expectation that you will help me tomorrow — may provide enough impetus for cooperation. But that works only if everyone knows one another and interacts frequently, says Muthukrishna. When a group gets big enough that people often interact with someone they’ve never dealt with before, reputation can substitute for direct experience. In these conditions, individuals are more likely to risk cooperating with others who have a reputation for doing their share.

Once a group gets so large that people can no longer count on knowingsomeone’s reputationthough, cooperation depends on a less personal force: the informal rules of behavior known as norms. Norms represent a culture’s expectations about how one should behave, how one should and shouldn’t act. Breaking a norm — whether by littering, jumping a subway turnstile or expressing overt racism — exposes violators to social disapproval that may range from a gentle “tut-tut” to social ostracism. People also tend to internalize their culture’s norms and generally adhere to them even when there is no prospect of punishment — as seen, for example, in the dictator game.

But there may be a limit to the power of norms, saysErez Joela behavioral scientist at the MIT Sloan School of Management. The enforcement of norms depends on social disapproval of violators, so they work only within social groups. Since nations are the largest groups that most people identify strongly with, that may make norms relatively toothless in developing international cooperation for issues such as climate change.

“The problem isn’t owned by a single group, so it’s kind of a race to the bottom,” says Yoeli. Social skills that go beyond cooperation, and psychological tools other than norms, may be more important in working through global problems, he speculates. “These are the ones we struggle a bit to solve.”

(Credit: RANDY FATH / UNSPLASH)A reputation for cooperating — doing your share — is an important asset in human societies and helps drive communal efforts like this Amish barn-raising.

Reputation is more powerful than financial incentives in encouraging cooperation

Almost a decade ago, Yoeli and his colleagues trawled through the published literature to see what worked and what didn’t at encouraging prosocial behavior. Financial incentives such as contribution-matching or cash, or rewards for participating, such as offering T-shirts for blood donors, sometimes worked and sometimes didn’t, they found. In contrast, reputational rewards — making individuals’ cooperative behavior public —consistently boosted participation. The result has held up in the years since. “If anything, the results are stronger,” says Yoeli.

Financial rewards will work if you pay people enough, Yoeli notes — but the cost of such incentives could be prohibitive. One study of 782 German residents, for example, surveyed whether paying people to receive a Covid vaccine would increase vaccine uptake. It did, but researchers found thatboosting vaccination rates significantly would have required a payment of at least 3,250 euros— a dauntingly steep price.

And payoffs can actually diminish the reputational rewards people could otherwise gain for cooperative behavior, because others may be unsure whether the person was acting out of altruism or just doing it for the money. “Financial rewards kind of muddy the water about people’s motivations,” says Yoeli. “That undermines any reputational benefit from doing the deed.”

Gossip plays a lead role in enforcing norms

When people see someone breaking a norm — for example, by freeloading when cooperation was expected — they have three ways to punish the violation, saysCatherine Saucea psychologist at Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam: They can confront the offender directly about their transgression; they can shun that person in the future; or they can tell others about the offender’s bad behavior. The latter response — gossip, or the sharing of information about a third party when they are not present — may have unique strengths, says Molho.

The clearest example of this comes from an online experiment led by Molho’s colleaguePaul Van Langea behavioral scientist also at Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam. The study used a standard lab procedure called a public goods game, in which each participant receives a sum of money and can choose to contribute none, some or all of it to a shared pool. The experimenters then double the money in the shared pool and divide it equally among all participants, whether they contributed or not. The group as a whole maximizes their earnings if everyone puts all their money in the pool — but a freeloader could do even better, by keeping their own cash and reaping a share of what others put in the pool.

Crucially, people played the game not just once but four times, with different partners each time. Between rounds, some participants had an opportunity to punish freeloaders from their most recent group by paying some of their own money to the experimenters, who would fine the freeloader three times the amount of that payment. Others were given the chance to gossip — that is, to tell members of the freeloaders’ new group that they had failed to cooperate. Sure enough, thegossip led to higher levels of cooperation— but, surprisingly, direct punishment did not, the researchers found.

(Credit: Knowable Magazine) In each round of a repeated “public goods game,” participants had a chance to contribute some or all of their funds for the common good, or selfishly keep it for themselves. People contributed more when others had an opportunity to gossip between rounds about participants’ earlier behavior, researchers found.

People use the power of gossip in the real world, too. In one recent study, Molho and her colleagues texted 309 volunteers at four random times each day for 10 days to ask if they had shared information with others in their social network, or received information from them, about someone else. If so, a follow-up questionnaire gathered more information.

The 309 participants reported more than 5,000 total instances of gossip over that time, and about 15 percent were about norm violations such as tossing trash in the street or making racist or sexist comments. People tended to gossip more with closer friends, and about more distant acquaintances. Gossip recipients reported that this negative information made themless likely to help the untrustworthy and more likely to avoid them.

“One reason gossip is such a powerful tool is you can accomplish many social functions,” says Molho. “You feel closer to the person who shared information with you. But we also find it provides useful information for social interaction — I learn who to cooperate with and who to avoid.”

And gossip serves another function, too, says Van Lange: Gossipers can sort through their feelings about whether a norm violation is important, whether there were mitigating circumstances and what response is appropriate. This helps reinforce the social norms and can help people coordinate their response to offenders, he says.

We like being on trend — and on the cutting edge

Some well-meaning ways of encouraging cooperation don’t work — and may even backfire. In particular, telling people what others actually do (“Most people are trying to reduce how often they fly”) is more effective than telling them what they should do (“You should fly less — it’s bad for the climate”). In fact, the “should” message sometimes backfires. “People may read something behind the message,” says&n bsp;Christine Bicchieria behavioral scientist at the University of Pennsylvania: Telling someone they should do something may signal that people don’t, in fact, do it.

Bicchieri and her colleague Erte Xiao tested this in a dictator game where some participants were told that other people shared equally, while others were told that people thought everyone should share equally. Only thefirst message increased the likelihood of an equal sharethey found.

That result makes sense, says Yoeli. “It sends a very clear message about social expectations: If everybody else is doing this, it sends a very credible signal about what they expect me to do.”

(Credit:Knowable Magazine)In a classic study from 2008, researchers tested different messages to encourage hotel guests to reuse their towels instead of getting freshly laundered ones each day. The message that referred to how most people behave in the same situation led more guests to take this environmentally friendly action.

This poses a problem, of course, if most people don’t actually choose a socially desirable behavior, such as installing solar panels. “If you just say that 15 percent do that, you normalize the fact that 85 percent don’t,” says Bicchieri. But there’s a work-around: It turns out that even a minority can nudge people toward a desired behavior if the number is increasing, thus providing a trendy bandwagon to hop on. In one experiment, for example, researchers measured the amount of water volunteers used while brushing their teeth. People who had been told that a small butincreasing proportion of peoplewere conserving water used less water than those who heard only that a small proportion conserved.

Much remains unknown

Behavioral scientists are just beginning to crack the problem of cooperation, and many questions remain. In particular, very little is known yet about why cultures hold the norms that they do, or how norms change over time. “There’s a lot of ideas about the within-group processes that cause norms to be replaced, but there’s not much consensus,” says Boyd, who is working on the problem now.Everyone does agree that, eventually, natural selection will determine the outcome, as cultures whose norms do not enhance survival die out and are replaced by those with norms that do. But that’s not a test most of us would be willing to take.

10.1146/knowable-032923-1


Bob Holmesis a science writer based in Edmonton, Canada. This article originally appeared inKnowable Magazinean independent journalistic endeavor from Annual Reviews. You can read the original here.

Note: This article have been indexed to our site. We do not claim legitimacy, ownership or copyright of any of the content above. To see the article at original source Click Here

Related Posts
L3 Harris wins $765.5 million contract to develop GeoXO imager thumbnail

L3 Harris wins $765.5 million contract to develop GeoXO imager

The National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration's Geostationary and Extended Orbits constellation includes three satellites: GEO-West, GEO-Center and GEO-East. Credit: NOAA WASHINGTON — L3Harris Technologies won a $765.5 million NASA contract to develop the imager for the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration’s Geostationary Extended Observations satellite program. The cost-plus-award-fee contract for the GeoXO imager, known as
Read More
What is the biggest star ever observed? thumbnail

What is the biggest star ever observed?

Home References Science & Astronomy The biggest star is UY Scuti, about 1,700 times larger than the sun. (Image credit: Philip Park (CC BY-SA 3.0)) Our sun is enormous: More than a million Earths could fit inside of it. But on a stellar scale, it could be swallowed up by about half of all stars…
Read More
Genetic study links blood sugar and type 2 diabetes treatment thumbnail

Genetic study links blood sugar and type 2 diabetes treatment

Groundbreaking research in Nature Genetics unveils the biggest-ever study on the genetics of random blood sugar levels that fluctuate throughout the day and night. This extensive study involved nearly half a million people from diverse backgrounds. It has identified new genetic variations that impact these random blood sugar levels.  The research team, led by Professor
Read More
Beloved Aṉangu Country given heritage listing thumbnail

Beloved Aṉangu Country given heritage listing

A national park in the Red Centre that's home to Kings Canyon and an important Aṉangu cultural site has been placed on the federal heritage list.Watarrka National Park, 300km west of Alice Springs, sits on the traditional lands of the Aṉangu people.The group has a deep connection to the site and has maintained area's the
Read More
ANA's 787, 10 years since its first receipt in the world Special feature: Dreamliner from the perspective of a mechanic thumbnail

ANA's 787, 10 years since its first receipt in the world Special feature: Dreamliner from the perspective of a mechanic

 2011年9月28日に全日本空輸(ANA/NH)のボーイング787型機(787-8、登録記号JA801A)が羽田空港に初めて降り立ってから10年が過ぎた。米シアトル近郊にあるエバレット工場で現地時間25日午前8時(日本時間26日午前0時)に、ローンチカスタマーであるANAへ引き渡され、28日の羽田到着時には多くの人が展望デッキで出迎えた。 787の前に並ぶ(左から)ANAの川井マネジャー、浅井マネジャー、青島マネジャー。787の世界初受領から10年を迎えた=PHOTO: Tadayuki YOSHIKAWA/Aviation Wire  ボーイングが「ドリームライナー」と名付けた787は現在、標準型の787-8、長胴型の787-9、超長胴型の787-10の3機種があり、ANAは全機種を導入。9月28日現在、787-8を36機、787-9を37機、787-10を2機の計75機を受領済みで、ボーイングの受注残によると8機の787-9と12機の787-10の計20機が受領待ちとなっている。  機体の構造部位のうち、35%を日本企業を製造。ANAの787はラバトリー(化粧室)に温水便座「ウォシュレット」を採用した。外観も初号機と2号機(JA802A)は特別塗装となり、前部胴体に「787」と大きく描き、後部の藍色に交差するラインは「ANAのネットワーク」と「ANAのプロダクトサービスブランド」を表現した。 羽田空港に着陸するANAの787初号機JA801A=11年9月28日 PHOTO: Tadayuki YOSHIKAWA/Aviation Wire  世界初の787による商業運航は、2011年10月26日に成田を出発した香港行きチャーターのNH7871便。その後定期便に投入され、国内線は11月1日の羽田発岡山行きNH651便、国際線は2012年1月14日の羽田発北京行きNH1255便が初便となった。  世界で最初に引き渡されたANAの787は、自動車の車検に例えられる機体のCチェック(重整備)も最初に行われ、2014年6月から7月にかけて行われた。Cチェックはおおむね1年半から2年ごとに行われ、2回目は2017年4月から5月に実施しされ、直近では2020年2月から3月にかけて3回目のCチェックを終えている。  胴体がCFRP(炭素繊維複合材料)製になり、777など既存機と比べてコンピューター制御の部分も増えた787。新技術が数多く採用された機体を見守ってきた整備士たちには、どのような10年だったのだろうか。 —記事の概要— ・壊れないCFRP ・ソフトウェアも安定 ・就航後も改修提案 壊れないCFRP  CFRPは機体の軽量化に貢献しているが、整備作業では従来のアルミとは違った作業や点検が必要だ。例えば空港で駐機中、貨物コンテナを積み込む車両などがぶつかるケースがある。アルミは板金で対処できたが、CFRPは見た目だけでは損傷を受けていないように見える場合もある。 14年に行われた世界初の787のCチェック=14年6月 PHOTO: Tadayuki YOSHIKAWA/Aviation Wire ANAの浅井マネジャー=PHOTO: Tadayuki YOSHIKAWA/Aviation Wire  整備士として787の領収検査に立ち会ってきたANAの整備センター 機体事業室 ドック整備部の浅井祐樹マネジャーは、「衝撃に弱いため、中で剥離している場合があります。アルミはコロージョン(腐食)を見ていましたが、CFRPはコロージョンがないので、クラック(割れ目)がないかを確認しています」と、既存機とは異なる視点で点検する必要があるという。  補修作業が発生した場合も気を遣う。整備センター 技術部 技術企画チームの川井渉マネジャーは「CFRPは接着が難しく、温度管理や気泡が入らないように作業しています」と、素材の特性上やり直しを何度もできない難しさがあるという。  浅井さんは「CFRPは普通壊れません。壊れてしまうと大変なんです」と話す。ドック整備部の青島誠マネジャーも、「CFRPは温度と湿度の管理が必要で、非破壊検査の要求もかなり厳しいです」と、壊れない素材ゆえに損傷を受けた際の補修は大変なようだ。 ソフトウェアも安定  787はこれまで油圧制御だったものが電気制御に置き換わった部分も多く、コンピューター制御も増えた。「油圧ではエア抜きや正常に動作するか、油漏れがないかといった点検が主でした。787は電気系統も正しくつながっているかの点検も加わっています」(浅井さん)と、動作確認の範囲が広がった。 2回目のCチェックで水平尾翼の整備作業が進むANAの787初号機JA801A=17年4月 PHOTO: Tadayuki YOSHIKAWA/Aviation Wire ANAの川井マネジャー=PHOTO: Tadayuki YOSHIKAWA/Aviation Wire  主翼にあるスポイラーの場合、片側7枚中2枚が電気で動くエレキスポイラーになり、残り5枚は油圧制御だ。油圧も進化し、昔よりも小型化しているという。  点検作業の範囲が広がった一方で、整備士を支援する仕組みも充実している。「テストはすごく進化しています。CMC(セントラル・メンテナンス・コンピューター)でテスト項目を選択すると接続なども確認してくれます。テストにパスしなかった場合は、どこが悪かったかをワイヤリング(配線図)で追ったりします」(浅井さん)。  コンピューター制御が進んだ787を、川井さんは「パソコンと一緒ですね。自動車だと(電気自動車の)テスラみたいなものでしょうか」と表現した。「整備士がマニュアルに従って作業しても直らず、ソフトウェアの問題だったこともあります」(川井さん)と、ソフトウェアの不具合に起因する問題もあるという。  浅井さんは「今までは故障した物を見て原因がわかったことも、整備士と技術部門のスタッフ、メーカーが連携して対処することが多くなりました」と、問題を解決する方法にも変化があった。  「以前は不具合を知らせるメッセージが出ても正常なケースがあったのですが、ソフトウェアの改修が進んで安定してきました」(浅井さん)と、10年で熟成が進んだ。 就航後も改修提案  機体の構造や客室内の装備も、運航していく中でボーイングに改修を提案していったものもあった。…
Read More
Index Of News
Total
0
Share