This bird is like a GPS for honey

Show me the honey —

The honeyguide recognizes calls made by different human groups.

A bird perched on a wall in front of an urban backdrop.

Enlarge / A greater honeyguide

With all the technological advances humans have made, it may seem like we’ve lost touch with nature—but not all of us have. People in some parts of Africa use a guide more effective than any GPS system when it comes to finding beeswax and honey. This is not a gizmo, but a bird.

The Greater Honeyguide (highly appropriate name), Indicator indicator (even more appropriate scientific name), knows where all the beehives are because it eats beeswax. The Hadza people of Tanzania and Yao people of Mozambique realized this long ago. Hadza and Yao honey hunters have formed a unique relationship with this bird species by making distinct calls, and the honeyguide reciprocates with its own calls, leading them to a hive.

Because the Hadza and Yao calls differ, zoologist Claire Spottiswoode of the University of Cambridge and anthropologist Brian Wood of UCLA wanted to find out if the birds respond generically to human calls, or are attuned to their local humans. They found that the birds are much more likely to respond to a local call, meaning that they have learned to recognize that call.

Come on, get that honey

To see which sound the birds were most likely to respond to, Spottiswoode and Wood played three recordings, starting with the local call. The Yao honeyguide call is what the researchers describe as “a loud trill followed by a grunt (‘brrrr-hm’) while the Hadza call is more of “a melodic whistle,” as they say in a study recently published in Science. The second recording they would play was the foreign call, which would be the Yao call in Hadza territory and vice versa.

The third recording was an unrelated human sound meant to test whether the human voice alone was enough for a honeyguide to follow. Because Hadza and Yao voices sound similar, the researchers would alternate among recordings of honey hunters speaking words such as their names.

So which sounds were the most effective cues for honeyguides to partner with humans? In Tanzania, local Hadza calls were three times more likely to initiate a partnership with a honeyguide than Yao calls or human voices. Local Yao calls were also the most successful in Mozambique, where, in comparison to Hadza calls and human voices, they were twice as likely to elicit a response that would lead to a cooperative effort to search for a beehive. Though honeyguides did sometimes respond to the other sounds, and were often willing to cooperate when hearing them, it became clear that the birds in each region had learned a local cultural tradition that had become just as much a part of their lives as those of the humans who began it.

Now you’re speaking my language

There is a reason that honey hunters in both the Hadza and Yao tribes told Wood and Spottiswoode that they have never changed their calls and will never change them. If they did, they’d be unlikely to gather nearly as much honey.

How did this interspecies communication evolve? Other African cultures besides the Hadza and Yao have their own calls to summon a honeyguide. Why do the types of calls differ? The researchers do not think these calls came about randomly.

Both the Hadza and Yao people have their own unique languages, and sounds from them may have been incorporated into their calls. But there is more to it than that. The Hadza often hunt animals when hunting for honey. Therefore, the Hadza don’t want their calls to be recognized as human, or else the prey they are after might sense a threat and flee. This may be why they use whistles to communicate with honeyguides—by sounding like birds, they can both attract the honeyguides and stalk prey without being detected.

In contrast, the Yao do not hunt mammals, relying mostly on agriculture and fishing for food. This, along with the fact that they try to avoid potentially dangerous creatures such as lions, rhinos, and elephants, and can explain why they use recognizably human vocalizations to call honeyguides. Human voices may scare these animals away, so Yao honey hunters can safely seek honey with their honeyguide partners. These findings show that cultural diversity has had a significant influence on calls to honeyguides.

While animals might not literally speak our language, the honeyguide is just one of many species that has its own way of communicating with us. They can even learn our cultural traditions.

“Cultural traditions of consistent behavior are widespread in non-human animals and could plausibly mediate other forms of interspecies cooperation,” the researchers said in the same study.

Honeyguides start guiding humans as soon as they begin to fly, and this knack, combined with learning to answer traditional calls and collaborate with honey hunters, works well for both human and bird. Maybe they are (in a way) speaking our language.

Science, 2023.  DOI: 10.1126/science.adh412

Note: This article have been indexed to our site. We do not claim legitimacy, ownership or copyright of any of the content above. To see the article at original source Click Here

Related Posts
Eight countries jointly propose principles for mutual recognition of digital IDs thumbnail

Eight countries jointly propose principles for mutual recognition of digital IDs

Digital identity interoperability principles. Image: Digital Government Exchange Digital Identity Working Group Eight countries have jointly proposed a set of "high-level principles" in the hope they can enable future mutual recognition and interoperability of digital identities. The countries came together to form the digital identity working group (DIWG) in 2020 to discuss how to make…
Read More
Lenovo ThinkPad E14 G4 AMD reviewed: Affordable business laptop with expandable RAM convinces in many respects thumbnail

Lenovo ThinkPad E14 G4 AMD reviewed: Affordable business laptop with expandable RAM convinces in many respects

Lenovo ThinkPad E14 G4 AMD reviewed: Inexpensive, fast and quiet (Image: Notebookcheck)The inexpensive Lenovo ThinkPad E14 G4 AMD performs as well as a business laptop in almost all categories: the eight-core processor produces a lot of power, the fan is restrained and the keyboard is even better than in the current ThinkPad T-series. Nevertheless, real
Read More
Samsung develops solar watch straps, Galaxy Watch will have longer battery life thumbnail

Samsung develops solar watch straps, Galaxy Watch will have longer battery life

在今年早些時候,Samsung 就推出了一款 QLED 智能電視遙控器,而這款遙控器比較特殊,能夠利用室內光線、室外光線或 USB 進行充電。而該裝置所採用的太陽能技術也引發了不少消費者的關注,早前就有博主討論過這種太陽能技術是否適用於其他低功耗裝置上。而在近日,Samsung 給出了答案。據外媒 LetsGoDigital 報導顯示,美國專利和商標局(USPTO)9 月 16 日公佈了一項 Samsung 早在 2019年提交的專利申請。該專利的名稱為「由多種類型的太陽能電池組成的電子裝置」,共22頁。專利中描述了一款帶有太陽能錶帶的 Samsung Galaxy Watch ,兩個太陽能電池放置在錶帶中,在手錶外殼的兩側。並且在 LetsGoDigital 製作的產品渲染圖中可以看到,太陽能電池的位置有一個蜂窩狀結構,但在現實產品上你是看不到它的。值得一提的是,該專利並沒有詳細說明該系統的效率如何,但據猜測,Samsung 的想法是通過在錶殼兩側放置太陽能電池,無論如何佩戴手錶,都能收集到足夠的陽光。 Samsung 希望使用了一種帶有量子點的聚合物來改變光路,並使用幾種可以進行光電轉換的太陽能電池。太陽能電池捕捉通過聚合物的光線,之後將光線轉化為電能。即使它們的功率不足以為其完全供電,但也能起到提供輔助電源的作用。雖然 Samsung 並沒有說明該專利一定會成真,但 Samsung 在太陽能技術領域以及量子點的應用方面已經有充分的經驗,或許在不久的將來,一款能使用太陽能供電的 Galaxy 智能手錶將出現在我們的面前。
Read More

Russia arrests prominent cyber security executive

Founder and CEO of cyber security firm Group-IB detained in Moscow on treason charges By Alex Scroxton, Security Editor Published: 29 Sep 2021 16:45 Russian authorities have arrested Ilya Sachkov, the founder and chief executive of cyber security firm Group-IB, accusing him of collaborating with western intelligence agencies and treason, according to reports. RTVI, a…
Read More
Index Of News